Perruchet, P. & Pacton, S. (2006) Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5).
People have a distinct ability to detect patterns and this ability has been explained using two theories: implicit learning and statistical learning. Implicit learning theory suggests that after sufficient exposure to a pattern one automatically deduces a rule regarding the formation of that pattern which can then be accessed consciously. In contrast, statistical learning suggests no explicit rule learning is necessary and rather, that our brains perform statistical computations to predict the likelihood that a certain stimulus will occur given the occurrence of another stimulus. These theories have huge implications in the world of language acquisition, where words and grammatical structures must be intuited from continuous sequences of syllables.
In the present paper, Perruchet and Pacton (2006) make it clear that while the origins of implicit and statistical learning theories are vastly different, they have converged to a single goal: to explain general learning through domain-general processing. Further, Perruchet and Pacton (2006) suggest two explanations which attempt to integrate the two theories.
The first explanation suggests all levels of learning are done with chunks based on the frequency with which they occur. This model claims that statistical patterns, which statistical learning theory predicts are used to acquire information about a pattern, are only a byproduct of the frequency with which different chunks occur. The plausibility of this theory is supported by competitive chunking models like PARSER (Perruchet & Vinter, 1998, as cited in Perruchet and Pacton) which posits that chunks which maintain cognitive representation are those which are most frequent.
The second theory suggests that statistical computations are carried out to form the chunks and then these chunks are used in a competitive model. As Perruchet and Pacton explain, this explanation has the benefit of explaining why the stimulus preceding and following a string can impact the way it is remembered, a fact not well explained by competitive chunking models of learning like PARSER.
The paper concludes that there is insufficient evidence to support the superiority of either the pure chunking or the statistical chunking model of learning. It is suggested the distinction between statistical and implicit learning is important in determining the involvement of consciousness in learning, as implicit learning theory suggests chunks are manipulated consciously whereas statistical learning theory would indicate that chunk knowledge would be held in the unconscious.
Blogger; Braxton Murphy is an undergraduate research student working under the supervision of Dr. Lisa Archibald
This blog is maintained by the Language and Working Memory Laboratory at the University of Western Ontario. The purpose of this blog is to review research articles and discuss clinical implications. Please email our lab manager to request the original articles. Our lab manager can be contacted at lwmlab2505@gmail.com
Tuesday, May 16, 2017
Monday, May 8, 2017
Design-Based Research: Putting a Stake in the Ground
Learning sciences refers to
the study of learning and instructional methodologies. One approach to this
work is design-based research, the goal
of which is to create new theories, and practices impacting learning and
teaching in a real-life setting. In this approach, researchers systematically
asses the impact of changes to the learning context. Barab & Squire (2004) outline seven differences
between design-based research and traditional methodology. Some of these
distinctions include: the location of research, complexity of variables, focus
of research, and role of participants. These differences emphasize that design-research
often occurs in a real-life setting, that measurement is challenging due to the
continuously changing context, and that some researchers may be both designing
and participating in the study. A large
distinction between traditional research and design-research is that design-research
requires change at a local level and this change is used as evidence to support
the theory behind the design.
The
authors consider how to measure overall change in this approach and they pose
the question - what counts as credible research? In a design-based research
approach, the terms trustworthiness, credibility, and usefulness capture the
study’s reliability, validity, and generalizability/external validity. Some
critics of design-based research believe problems arise when the effectiveness
of design-based research is evaluated. This is because it is the researcher who
is determining the effectiveness that is also the designer and participated in
the interactions assessed. However, other researchers argue that design-research
can be adaptable to uniquely fit a local dynamic, and thereby the goal is to
develop flexible theories applicable to the current and new contexts.
Creating
design-research that is usable and sustainable when implemented in real-world
contexts may be an important facilitator of researcher-practitioner
collaboration. Design-researchers work together to provide credible,
trustworthy, and useful evaluation of instructional methods in a real-world
environment.
Blogger: Meghan Vollebregt is a
student in the combined SLP MSc/PhD program working under the supervision of
Lisa Archibald.
Wednesday, May 3, 2017
The role of language in mathematical development: Evidence from children with specific language impairments
Although language and mathematics
are distinct skills, some studies suggest that these two cognitive processes
are related. One view suggests that language plays a bootstrapping role in
numerical cognition, in which development of number concepts is dependent on
number-related language experience1. An alternate view proposes that
number concept development is independent of number word knowledge2.
Children with specific language impairment (SLI) have impairments in receptive
and expressive language but have non-verbal cognitive abilities within the
average range. By studying numerical cognition in children with SLI, the
present study aims to examine whether language impairments contribute to
difficulty in mathematical cognition. Specifically, the paper differentiates
between procedural mathematical knowledge (counting and basic calculations) and
conceptual mathematical knowledge (understanding of place value and arithmetic
principles) to examine the relative contributions of language and non-verbal
abilities to these aspects of numerical cognition.
The present study examined three
groups of children: children with SLI, typically developing children matched to
the SLI group based on age, and typically developing children matched to the
SLI group based on language comprehension. These participants all completed the
following tasks: counting aloud, simple calculations, multi-digit magnitude
comparison, and arithmetic problems using unfamiliar symbols. The counting and
calculations tasks were categorized as procedural tasks, while the magnitude
comparison and arithmetic problems tasks were categorized as conceptual as they
were designed to measure understanding of place value principles and arithmetic
principles.
Results demonstrated that on the
counting and calculation tasks, the SLI group performed similarly to the
language controls and more poorly than the age controls. On the magnitude
comparison tasks, the age controls outperformed the SLI group and the SLI group
outperformed the language controls. On the arithmetic principles task, the SLI
and age controls performed similarly and both outperformed the language
controls. Counting skills were a significant predictor of calculation and
magnitude comparison performance. These findings suggest that the children with
SLI are able to achieve conceptual understanding of mathematical principles,
but that their language weaknesses may contribute to difficulty developing
procedural mathematical skills. The authors suggest that conceptual
understanding of arithmetic may be supported by a system separate from language
but that language may support learning of the counting sequence, which, in
turn, supports understanding of calculation and number notation.
1. Carey, S. (2004).
Bootstrapping and the origin of concepts. Daedalus, 133, 59–68.
2. Gelman,
R., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Number and language: How are they related?
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 6–10.
Blogger: Alex
Cross is completing a combined MClSc and PhD in speech language pathology. Her
work focusing on reading will be part of both the Language and Working Memory
and the Language, Reading, and Cognitive Neuroscience labs.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)